Levels of visual pathway lesionsBefore the diagnostic observations in the functional assessment can be discussed, a short overview of the visual pathways is necessary to give a common ground to the discussion. During many international courses, visual pathways have been the most difficult chapter. Therefore the simplified pictures on the visual pathways were drawn for the "Vision Testing Manual". These pathways should be drawn by the reader as many times as is needed until their structure is quite clear. Otherwise it is too difficult to understand how a lesion might affect function. Before attempting a functional assessment or a clinical evaluation of a child's vision we should try to have a clear picture of the anatomy of the lesion(s).
Make photocopies of this drawing (PDF-file) for your training to draw visual pathways. The following schematic drawing shows the principal components of the visual pathways, as seen from the side of the head.
The ventral stream functions are sometimes called "what" functions, and the dorsal stream functions "where" functions. This is an oversimplification because we need recognition of the environment to orient in it although during the movement we do not analyse in detail the structure of the body part moving or the environment. The two main types of vision combine their functions and support them with information from visual memory. Visual pathway transfers different types of visual information from the retina via different nerve fibers. Parvocellular (P) fibers transfer colour and high contrast black-and-white detail information. Magnocellular fibers (M) transfer motion information and low contrast black-and-white information. The dichotomy is present already at the retinal level, where cone cell and rod cell based vision affect each other. The presence of parallel visual pathways and functions is typical to the visual system. This feature makes it possible that some visual functions are lost or altered by a lesion and at the same time other visual functions remain normal. The case history and the current imaging techniques give enough information for a good start. In developing countries, assessment must be based on careful and detailed history taking, general clinical examination, ophthalmologic examination, if available, and on the observations during follow-up. When we make our observations on children's or adult persons' visual functioning, we should have this variation in size and location of pathway lesions clear in our minds. Pathway lesions are easily forgotten, when the child has a lesion in the eyes. However, for example, many children with ROP have greater perceptual than primary visual problems. When we list the most important details that we should know about a child we need to pay attention to both cortical and subcortical functions. In the functional evaluation the questions are thus:
Variation in the combinations of the different disorders and in their severity is great. At one extreme are the children whose handling of visual information is so chaotic that they tend not to use it at all. At the other extreme are children with changes in one recognition function, an isolated agnosia, that can be identified by careful history taking and observation but the disorder does not affect the child's functioning other than in that specific task. The variation in the changes in the other sensory and motor functions and memory is equally great. Some children have delays in cognitive development or psychiatric disease complicating the picture. In some children visual experiences do not seem to be pleasant, they appear not to enjoy looking at things . This phenomenon is known in adult patients with neurological disorders who after circulatory failures in the brain stem sometimes experience a loss of pleasure in looking. Such a loss may last for months. If a similar lesion has occurred in a child who therefore does not experience pleasure when using vision, vision cannot function as the driving force as it does in infants and children with normal brain function. How often we have this problem in children who do not show interest in looking at things, is not possible to guess. The function of looking at things is a specific function that requires directing attention to the object. If there is a deficit in directing attention to visual information, the child does not pause to look at some specific features, the gaze glides from one object to another. This feature is also difficult to diagnose with certainty. It should not be confused with lack of focused looking caused by insufficient accommodation. Children are not routinely assessed for their accommodation although in many cases it is possible to measure whether there is any change in refraction when an interesting object is moved closer and farther away. Therefore, always, when a child does not seem to look at things or focusing does not seem to be adequate, but the child seems to look through, not at the object, the effect of plus lenses on the child's behaviour needs to be investigated. Autistic features are often inappropriately mentioned as a feature of children with CVD. They may have true autistic features but in a number of cases the diagnosis is based on misinterpretation of the child's visual behaviour. If a child cannot direct gaze at another person but the gaze wanders on both sides of him/her without eye contact, it may be inappropriately diagnosed as autistic behaviour by someone who is not familiar with the child's problem. Similarly, a child who uses eccentric fixation and seems to look past the person - often at or above the hairline - and not at the person to whom he/she talks, may be incorrectly labeled as "avoiding eye contact". A child who does not accommodate to focus on the other person's face, seems to look through but not at the person and this behaviour may again be incorrectly labeled as an autistic feature. Before the assessment, we should discuss the behaviour of the child in detail. Children with CVD have several behaviours in common. A particular child may have a few such features, while another may display nearly all of them, depending on which brain functions have been altered. Observations of parents, therapists and teachers can be collected by using a questionnaire covering the following aspects of behaviour:
Instead of using printed forms that the parents, therapists or teachers would fill in, it is advisable to collect this information during interviews. It would be a great improvement if these questions were asked at strabismus (squint) clinics because so many of these children are treated there without being diagnosed as having the more important deviations of brain functions. The compensatory techniques used in pre-school activities are not possible in school situations. Therefore school often becomes a great disappointment and misery to a child who was an eager learner in pre-school.
|